Acknowledgments |
|
xi | |
Introduction |
|
xv | |
|
Getting Philosophically Involved with Emotion |
|
|
1 | (20) |
|
|
1 | (3) |
|
|
4 | (5) |
|
|
9 | (1) |
|
|
10 | (2) |
|
The True Self; or, The Antinomy of Integrity |
|
|
12 | (2) |
|
The Indeterminism of Reason and Nature |
|
|
14 | (3) |
|
|
17 | (1) |
|
Emotions as a Philosophical Hub |
|
|
18 | (3) |
|
Models of Mind and Emotion |
|
|
21 | (26) |
|
|
21 | (3) |
|
|
24 | (6) |
|
|
30 | (6) |
|
Some Typical Modern Theories |
|
|
36 | (11) |
|
Emotion and Biology: Physiology and Function |
|
|
47 | (30) |
|
|
47 | (1) |
|
The Chemistry of Love: Emotion and Physiological Control |
|
|
48 | (1) |
|
Physiology and the Vocabulary of Emotional Description |
|
|
49 | (2) |
|
|
51 | (12) |
|
|
63 | (6) |
|
The Systems Approach: Top-Down Biology |
|
|
69 | (8) |
|
Evolution and Teleology: From Instinct to Intentionality |
|
|
77 | (30) |
|
|
77 | (3) |
|
Evolutionary Explanations |
|
|
80 | (5) |
|
Freudian Instinct: Determinism or Teleology? |
|
|
85 | (1) |
|
Teleology and Natural Selection |
|
|
86 | (5) |
|
|
91 | (1) |
|
|
92 | (4) |
|
|
96 | (1) |
|
The Importance of Singular Reference |
|
|
97 | (6) |
|
Emotions and the ``Biologically Natural'' |
|
|
103 | (2) |
|
|
105 | (2) |
|
Emotions and Their Objects |
|
|
107 | (34) |
|
|
107 | (2) |
|
What Are Objects? Six Problems |
|
|
109 | (4) |
|
|
113 | (1) |
|
|
114 | (9) |
|
Two Approaches to Canonical Emotion Ascriptions |
|
|
123 | (7) |
|
|
130 | (4) |
|
Nonexistent Objects Again |
|
|
134 | (3) |
|
Propositional Objects and Thought-Dependency |
|
|
137 | (4) |
|
The Rational and the Objective |
|
|
141 | (30) |
|
|
141 | (2) |
|
Truth, Objectivity, and Rationality |
|
|
143 | (6) |
|
The Analogy of Perception |
|
|
149 | (9) |
|
Six Principles of Rationality |
|
|
158 | (7) |
|
The Irreducibility of Emotion |
|
|
165 | (4) |
|
|
169 | (2) |
|
The Rationality of Emotion |
|
|
171 | (34) |
|
|
171 | (2) |
|
|
173 | (4) |
|
Subjective and Objective Desires |
|
|
177 | (3) |
|
|
180 | (1) |
|
|
181 | (3) |
|
The Principles of Rationality Applied |
|
|
184 | (2) |
|
|
186 | (4) |
|
What Are Emotions For? A New Biological Hypothesis |
|
|
190 | (6) |
|
|
196 | (5) |
|
Salience and Paradigm Scenarios: The Euthyphro Once More |
|
|
201 | (4) |
|
|
205 | (30) |
|
|
205 | (2) |
|
Desire and Temporal Aspect |
|
|
207 | (13) |
|
Temporal Transference and Temporal Discounting |
|
|
220 | (4) |
|
The Perspectival Relativity of Desire |
|
|
224 | (7) |
|
|
231 | (4) |
|
Pathologies of Bootstrapping: Self and Others |
|
|
235 | (30) |
|
|
235 | (1) |
|
How Much Can a Bootstrap Hoist? |
|
|
236 | (2) |
|
|
238 | (3) |
|
|
241 | (2) |
|
|
243 | (1) |
|
|
244 | (5) |
|
The Social Dimension of Emotions |
|
|
249 | (4) |
|
|
253 | (5) |
|
The Ideology of Emotions: Two Examples from the Natural History of Sexism |
|
|
258 | (3) |
|
The Dialectic of Fungibility |
|
|
261 | (1) |
|
|
262 | (3) |
|
|
265 | (10) |
|
True Love: A Meta-Lovers' Argument |
|
|
265 | (10) |
|
When Is It Wrong to Laugh? |
|
|
275 | (26) |
|
|
275 | (3) |
|
Three Arguments against Taking the Subject Seriously |
|
|
278 | (3) |
|
|
281 | (2) |
|
|
283 | (4) |
|
Feeling and Thinking: The Walberg View |
|
|
287 | (2) |
|
Phthonos, Wit, and Anhypothetical Humor |
|
|
289 | (3) |
|
|
292 | (1) |
|
Identification and Alienation: Inside and Outside |
|
|
293 | (2) |
|
An Axiological Perspective on Laughter |
|
|
295 | (2) |
|
|
297 | (4) |
|
Emotion and the Conduct of Life |
|
|
301 | (34) |
|
|
301 | (2) |
|
Ethics and the Avoidance of Emotion |
|
|
303 | (2) |
|
Are Some Emotions More Moral Than Others? |
|
|
305 | (9) |
|
The Expansion of the Ethical |
|
|
314 | (5) |
|
|
319 | (6) |
|
|
325 | (4) |
|
Three Basic Tragedies of Life |
|
|
329 | (2) |
|
|
331 | (4) |
Appendix Labeled Sentences and Principles Discussed |
|
335 | (4) |
Notes |
|
339 | (8) |
References |
|
347 | (12) |
Index |
|
359 | |